The
thoughts on emotions of conflict and emotions of bonding are my own design. They make sense to me but I am not making
them “black and white” or “either/or”.
The stuff on depression and despair are also my own. The definition I give of love is also my own.
Emotions have been categorized as
primary emotions and emotions of reference. The primary emotions come with the
package. Infants have these emotions.
Numerous researchers have listed these emotions. A hybrid list would be anger, sadness,
disgust, interest, joy, and distress or fear. These grow and mature across time
and become more shaped by the external environment. At base, these are our body
speaking to us.
Emotions of reference emerge with
the development of the self-consciousness of the self. This is the time around two years of age
where the mobile, verbal toddler is letting the I, Me, and Mine fly
around. (The term “emotions of reference”
comes from Michael Lewis’ book Shame.)
From the beginning of human life, membership in our social
group is all important. From here we find our meaning, our value, our
importance. Each individual human is
forming and evolving from the onset of life.
Self consciousness begins to clearly manifest with the child recognizing
herself in the mirror by the age of 18 months old. It is certainly off and running in the two
year old toddler with the developmental convergence of mobility, linguistic
pronoun use of “I”, “me” and “mine”, and the beginnings of what Michael Lewis
calls the emotions of reference; examples - pride, guilt, shame, jealousy. These emotions are measures of the self with
some social standard.
My social environment teaches me
the what, how, when, why, where, and who
of referencing my self. When a
perceived need arises to compare myself by looking at the advantages that age
or sex or hair color have in my family, my envy develops. I accomplish the spelling of a word with
great applause from my grandmother, my feelings and the incoming messages
converge into pride. These experiences
and the experience of all emotions of reference are my feelings of how I fit
into my social environment.
For a highly and imperatively social animal, our lifelong development of
attachment/bonding is pivotal to belonging.
We have a genetically based need to find structure, process and meaning
within a social context that arises from both our evolutionary path and the
very composition of our information processing.
It is an interplay of biology, language, family, society, culture, and
cosmology. It is a dynamic, ongoing,
relational process within ourselves and with others.
I choose the word belonging with its obvious anthropocentric overtones,
for three reasons. First, I look out
from human eyes with human concerns.
Second, I wanted a word that fit the transformation of the infant’s need
for attachment/bonding that occurs with linguistic acquisitions around two
years of age. Finally, with the adaptive
function of self consciousness having evolved to facilitate our fitting into
the social setting, the word belonging with its warmth and substance seemed to
capture this lifelong, dynamic process.
Lack of social support has been linked to cancer, heart disease,
arthritis, diseases of the immune system, addictions, as well as depression and
other forms of mental illness. A child in an orphanage can be given the best of
physical care. He can be kept clean and
well fed; but if he is not picked up and loved, he may lose weight and die. Among tribal people banishment from the
tribe can mean illness and/or death. This need not be actual physical
removal. Simply the loss of the social
support takes away the sense of place, time and meaning.
I believe the experience that wells up when we feel we do not belong is
shame. Shame is the experience of not
being acceptable in the social environment; of our personhood or an intimate
aspect of our humanity being ostracized.
As example, shame arises when we, especially as children, have no
socially acceptable release for our natural frustration/anger. Or where our natural feelings of flight
manifest as fear or terror are condemned.
Shame is the feeling that arises when a behavior that is manifesting a
naturally occurring internal state invokes the social response of disgust; of
being cast out; of not belonging.
With the social response imprinted very early on our basic survival
patterns, self-talk acts to maintain a sense of shame whenever the disallowed
internal experience occurs. This is
often below awareness because recognition of this aspect of our self is a
threat to belonging; hence to survival.
Shame’s counterpart is guilt. Guilt arises from the disapproval of our
behavior as opposed to rejection of our personhood. When guilt occurs, a way is taught for
rectifying our error and for the acceptable expression (no matter how
convoluted) of our experience within the social context. Guilt provides a process for continued
membership in the group. In this way it
provides continued support for the “traditional” patterns of socially accepted
behavior.
Shame and guilt are decidedly different experiences. Guilt offers continued membership while shame
banishes. The pathway to human belonging is channeled and powered by these two
emotions of reference that arise through the functioning of self
consciousness. I believe these two
emotions of reference are primary in the processes of personal and social
change.
Another way of dividing emotions
is possible. Although not mutually
exclusive, emotions can also be seen as expressions of either conflict or of bonding. Emotions of conflict describe situations that
need changing; situations that are threatening.
We feel fear at finding a snake in our path or perhaps we feel anger at
having been passed over for a promotion.
The emotions of conflict are designed in the body to persist until we
change the situation. The persistence of
conflict emotions until removal of the conflict originally had high survival
value. In the modern world so condensed
in time and space and with no safe outlets for these conflict emotions, this
persistence represents a serious problem of adaptation and health.
Excitement, joy, curiosity are
examples of emotions of bonding. These
emotions are designed not to persist.
They occur with nursing, with play, with laughter, with intimacy. For a highly social animal, the need to
regenerate bonding emotions by seeking situations that elicit them also has
survival value.
Metaphorically, conflict and
bonding emotions can be compared to fat soluable and water soluble vitamins. The fat soluble vitamins are necessary but
too much of them in the body are toxic.
The water-soluble vitamins pass out of the body very quickly and must be
replenished.
There is a further corollary
involving emotions of conflict and bonding.
Most of us have known the persistence of fear, sadness or anger in our
life. These emotions were originally
designed to persist. They may persist in humans because of psychohistorical
events. Then when we feel joy and it does not persist there is a tendency to blame
ourselves and feel like we are failures.
We are being unfair to ourselves.
We are comparing apples to oranges.
Emotions are timeless. When we tap into our sadness, we tap into all
the sadness we have ever or will ever feel.
This is a hard one for cause/effect types like myself. It is easier to appreciate by separating the
content of a sad event from the unique bodily experience of sadness. The content of sad events will vary in the
future; not our personal, internal domain of sadness.
We have joy within us as a natural
response. When we experience joyfulness, we move into a sensual mind field that
is neurological and biochemical. Within
this field we move outside the realm of clock time. When we feel joy we open ourselves to our
universe of joy.
In the broadest sense, our emotional reactions
to the continuing events of our life are the thread that gives us a personal
sense of history. Emotions give meaning
to experience in the moment and connect us to our memories. Our memories give us our history. Emotions shape our experience of the
self.
Let me add one final point. Love is not one emotion; it is all
emotions. A white light when passed
through a prism becomes a rainbow of colors.
Metaphorically, love is the white light of emotions. When passing through us, love is the many
hues and blends of the various feelings socially shaped and expressed as
emotions.
This perhaps gives us some clarity
as to why it is so difficult to love ourself and others. Love is all the emotions. Emotions are timeless. With love, we tap into not only our joy but
also our fear and sadness from now, before, and in the future. At times this makes it painful to have love
for ourselves and/or others.
When our emotions are affirmed,
nurtured and guided, the structures and meanings of the developing self are
connected internally and socially. There is belonging.
Love is the energy of life speaking to
itself. We are the crystal and love
comes from within us and through us.
Love, as all the emotions, as the white light, stands as the grail of
human spiritual endeavors. To love is to
celebrate living. It takes great
courage.
Depression
and despair are the handmaidens of shame anger.
They arise unbidden. Each of the
people we have looked at experiences these two sides of the same coin when the
energy from the shame within them overwhelms.
Depression
is a helplessness. It is a feeling of
not inadequacy but no adequacy. Like
being in a huge vat of mud and not being able to move fast enough. It is not being able to change the
circumstances or environment or behaviors.
It has been called anger turned inward.
If it were turned outward it would be aimed at the circumstances/environment/behaviors
(persons) that are creating the helplessness. Depression is related to the
past. It arises when all our adaptations fail to relieve energy of our
shame. The energy of depression is
pervasive and is a mood and in the gut.
Despair
has to do with hopelessness. This relates even more strongly to time. It has to do with the future. It is a fear of
a continuation of the present situation.
It is same old, same old. It is the feeling that there is no freedom
from the repetition of the patterns of adaptation. These patterns are unsuccessful
in getting our needs met and they will go on and on and on. So the future is bleak and hopeless.
Depression
rises out of the gut from the energy of a failed past and flows into the head
dampening action. Despair arise out of
the mind because the idea of a future is in the head. Despair flows into the gut dampening the
emotional energy for action. They are
truly the two headed face of shame in control.
Bibliography
Conlan, Roberta (editor). 1999. States
of Mind. Wiley. NY.
Fossum, Merle and Mason, M. 1986. Facing
Shame: Families in Recovery. Norton.
N.Y.
Gilbert, Paul and Andrews, B. 1998. Shame:Interperson
Behavior, Psycholpathology, and Culture.
Oxford U. Press. Oxford.
Gruen, Arno. 1988. The
Betrayal of the Self. Grove Press.
N. Y.
Lewis, H.B. 1987. The
Role of Shame in Symptom Formation.
Eribaum Ass. Hillsdale, N.J.
Lewis, H.B. 1971. Shame
and Guilt in Neurosis. International
University Press. N.Y.
Lewis, Michael. 1992. Shame-The
Exposed Self. The Free Press. N.Y.
Lowen. 1985. Narcissism: Denial
of the True Self.
Luria, A. R. 1973. “The Frontal Lobes and the Regulation of
Behavior.” In Psychophysiology of the
Frontal Lobes. Edited by K. Pribram
and A. Luria. Academic Press. N. Y.
Lynd, Helen Merrell. 1965. On
Shame and the Search for Identity.
Science Editions. N.Y.
Masterson, James. 1988. The
Search for the Real Self. Free
Press. N.Y.
Middelton-Moz, J. 1990. Shame
and Guilt. Health
Communications. Deerfield Beach, FL.
Miller, Alice. Drama of the Gifted Child
Peristiany, J. G. 1966. Honour
and Shame. University of Chicago
Press. Chicago.
Potter-Efron, Ronald and
Potter-Efron, Patricia. 1989. Letting Go of Shame. Harper. N.Y.
Pound, A. 1982.
"Attachment and Maternal Depression." In The Place of Attachment in Human
Behavior. Edited by C. . Parkes and
J.
Stevenson-Hinde. Basic Books.
N.Y. Pages118-130.
Pribam, K. 1984. "Emotion: A Neurobehavioral
Analysis. In Approaches to
Emotions. Edited by Scherer, K;
Ekman, P. Lawrence Erlbaum. London.
Pages 13-38
Pribam, K. and Luria, A. (editors)
1973. Psychophysiology of the Frontal
Lobes. Academic Press.
Scheff, T. and Retzinger, S. 1991. Emotions and Violence: Shame and
Rage in Destructive Conflicts. Lexington Books. Massachusetts>
Schieffelin, C. 1985.
"Anger, Grief, and Shame: Toward a Kaluli
Ethnopsychology." In Person,
Self, and Experience. Edited by G.
M. White and J. Kirkpatrick. Univ. of
California Press. Berkeley.
Schneider, Carl. 1977. Shame,
Exposure, Privacy. Beacon. Boston.
Schore, Alan ( 1999). Affect regulation and the origin
of the self: The neurobiology of emotional development. Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates. Hillsdale, New Jersey.
Vaillant, George. 1974. Adaptation to Life. Little, Brown, and Co. Boston.
No comments:
Post a Comment